The goal of this research was to determine the
relationships between the social response of insects
similar to honey bees and the external environmental
factors of water vapor and smoke. The first two
experiments were aimed at finding the relationship
between the presence of water vapor and alarm
pheromone response in house crickets. We tested
whether an alarm response could be induced by
exposure to volatile acetic acid (household vinegar), which is a component of an alarm pheromone. After
confirming this response, experiments were conducted
to see if water vapor could alter or block this response.
Experiment 1 (Figure 1) tested the prompting of an
alarm pheromone reaction using volatile acetic acid. The
crickets were divided into two groups of ten each. The
control group was not exposed to any outside trigger and
the experimental group was exposed to volatile acetic
acid. Each trial lasted for a period of 15 minutes, and the
alarm pheromone response was measured according
to a 0–5 scale for activity, as specified in the Materials
and Methods section. Each trial had an experimental
group and a control group. There were two trials, and
in order to ensure the group itself was not simply more
active, they switched between the role of the control and
experimental group.
This experiment tested the relationship between the
prompt of the alarm pheromone (volatile acetic acid) and
the social behavior of the crickets. As seen in Figure 1,
the activity levels in the experimental groups are higher
than those of the control groups. The experimental
groups experienced a downward trend over time,
suggesting that the volatile acetic acid escaped from
the container, thereby causing a decrease in the alarm
pheromone response. The control group experienced
a change in behavior that was significantly less than
that of the experimental group. The results of this first
experiment provided evidence that the acetic acid does,
in fact, trigger an alarm response in the house crickets.
With this relationship defined, Experiment 2 was
conducted to test the ability of the water vapor to block
the alarm pheromone social response. It was expected
that the water vapor could interfere with the pheromone
communication system (artificially prompted as in
Experiment 1). During Experiment 2 the control groups
were exposed to volatile acetic acid alone, while the
experimental group was exposed to acetic acid in the
presence of water vapor. The crickets were in larger
containers and the amount of acetic acid was increased
proportionally. Experiment 2 consisted of two trials, and
the control group and experimental group of ants were
switched between the trials, just as in experiment 1.
As seen by observing the changes in activity levels
in the house crickets that were treated with both volatile
acetic acid and water vapor (Figure 2), it can be
concluded that the alarm pheromone social response
decreased when the water vapor was present. The
activity in the control group of this experiment was
consistent with that of the crickets exposed to volatile
acetic acid in Experiment 1. The crickets had a high
activity level when there was acetic acid in the air of their
container. The activity of the house crickets treated with
both acetic acid and water vapor was lower, suggesting
that the alarm pheromone social response was by
blocked by the water vapor.
In the Experiment 3 which followed, it was predicted
that the smoke sources in the environment, including
smoke from the common practice of smoking bees
to control them and smoke from pollution, could be
harmful to insects. This study tested whether or not
smoke accumulates on the surfaces of insect bodies. To
do this, red harvester ants were used and split into an
experimental group and a control group. For the first part
of the experiment (Figures 3 and 4) the experimental
group was given three doses of smoke per day for a
total of eight days. The smoke was created by burning
paper, leaves, and wood: all sources of smoke in the
environment that are created by bee smokers or pollution.
The ants’ bodies were analyzed under a microscope
every two days. The smoke particle abundance was
measured for different parts of the bodies of the ants.
These observations were made approximately three
hours after each smoke application.
Over the period of eight days of observation, the
smoke particle abundance increased in most parts of
the ants’ bodies in the experimental group (Figure 3).
The trends present in the experimental group were
visible in almost all body parts with the exception of the
ants’ eyes and legs. The changes over time in smoke
particle abundance did not occur at the same rate for all
the parts of the body observed. This is in part because
of ant behavior, such as the ants rubbing their antennae
after the smoke dose was given. This suggests that the
ants removed some of the smoke particles themselves.
The control ants did have some smoke particles on the
surfaces of their body parts; however, they were not as
high as the levels in the experimental group (Figure 4).
They also did not change over time.
Because previous observations had been taken up
to four hours after the smoke doses had been given, a
separate experiment was done to determine the smoke
particle abundance directly after the smoke dose was
given. It was expected that this would give a more
accurate picture of how smoke sticks to the surfaces of
ants’ bodies without the factor of the ants trying to clean
the smoke particles from their bodies. This was tested in
Experiment 4 (Figure 5). Ants were given a smoke dose
and then the smoke particle abundances on their bodies
were observed directly after the dose was given.
The smoke particle abundance levels measured
directly after the smoke dose show higher levels than in
the experimental group’s last measurements (Experiment
3) on most parts of the body. The most interesting part
of the data is that the antennae had a much higher level
of smoke particles present directly after the dose. This
confirms that it was the ants themselves that had, with
their own legs, removed some of the smoke particles
from the antennae. The smoke particle abundance levels
in the eyes, however, were not changed, suggesting that
the smoke particles do not easily stick to the surface of
the eyes. In addition to this test, the abundance of various
sizes of smoke particles was found. This was done by
taking the average abundance of particles that were
binned by size. The abundances of the different sized
particles were measured and it was found that the most
abundantly-visible particles were of 175 micrometers in
diameter.